Normative Data and Construct Validation for a Novel Nonverbal Memory Test

Authors

  • Amir M Poreh Cleveland State University / Case Western University Medical Center
  • Max Teaford

Abstract

The present study describes the normative data and convergent validity of the Poreh Spatial Memory Test (PSMT). 204 participants (96 males and 108 females) between the age of 18 to 85 years were administered the PSMT. A subsample was administered the Rey Osterith Complex Figure Test (ROCFT) as well as the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) and another subsample was administered the Biber Figure Learning Test – Extended (BFLT-E). The present study demonstrated that the mean scores for the learning trials within each group exhibited robust inverse logarithmic learning curves. Accordingly, three indices were derived: learning, learning curve, and delayed retention. As one might expect from a “pure†visuospatial memory test, age, but not gender or education, correlated with the PSMT learning and delayed retention scores. Additional analysis revealed that the PSMT correlated significantly with the ROCFT and the BFLT-E, supporting the convergent validity of the new measure. Age based norms for this new measure were computed for the learning and delayed retention subscores, using a regression based procedure. Additional studies using various clinical samples including patients with lateralized brain injuries are underway to further establish the sensitivity and specificity of the PSMT.

Author Biography

Amir M Poreh, Cleveland State University / Case Western University Medical Center

Professor

References

Astur, R. S., St. Germain, S. A., Baker, E. K., Calhoun, V., Pearlson, G. D., & Constable, R. D. (2005). fMRI Hippocampal Activity During a Virtual Radial Arm Maze. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 30(3), 307-317. doi:10.1007/s10484-005-6385-z

Bertholet, L., Escobar, M. T., Depré, M., Chavan, C. F., Giuliani, F., Gisquet-Verrier, P., . . . Schenk, F. (2015). Spatial radial maze procedures and setups to dissociate local and distal relational spatial frameworks in humans. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 253, 126–141. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.06.012

Bouman, Z., Elhorst D., Hendriks, M., Kessels, R. & Aldenkamp, A. P.(2016). Clinical utility of the Wechsler Memory Scale - Fourth Edition (WMS-IV) in patients with intractable temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 55, 178-182. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.11.022.

Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., & O'Keefe, J. (2002). The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron, 35(4), 625–641. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9

Daugherty , A. M., Yuan, P., Dahle, C. L., Bender, A. R., Yang, Y., & Raz, N. (2015). Path complexity in virtual water maze navigation: Differential associations with age, sex, and regional brain volume. Cerebral Cortex, 25(9), 3122-3131. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu107

Fajnerová, I., Rodriguez, M., Levcík, D., Konrádová, L., Mikoláš, P., Brom, C., . . . Horácek, J. (2014). A virtual reality task based on animal research –Spatial learning and memory in patients after the first episode of schizophrenia. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 1-15. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00157

Feigenbaum, J. D., & Morris, R. G. (2004). Allocentric versus egocentric spatial memory after unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. Neuropsychology, 18(3), 462-472. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.462

Gallagher, M., & Rapp, P. R. (1997). The use of animal models to study the effects of aging on cognition. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 48, 339-370. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.339

Glosser, G., Cole, L., Khatri, U., DellaPietra, L., & Kaplan, E. (2002). Assessing nonverbal memory with the Biber Figure Learning Test—Extended in temporal lobe epilepsy patients. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17(1), 25–35. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(00)00093-7

Guárdia-Olmos, J., Peró-Cebollero, M., Rivera, D., & Arango-Lasprilla, J. C. (2015). Methodology for the development of normative data for ten Spanish-language neuropsychological tests in eleven Latin American countries. NeuroRehabilitation, 37(4), 493-499. doi:10.3233/NRE-151277

Higa, K. K., Young, J. W., & Geyer, M. A. (2016). Wet or dry: Translatable “water mazes†for mice and humans. The Journal of Clinical Investigation, 126(2), 477-479. doi:10.1172/JCI86071

Hodges, H. (1996). Maze procedures: the radial-arm and water maze compared. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(3-4), 167–181. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(96)00004-3

Lavenex, P. B., & Lavenex, P. (2010). Spatial relational learning and memory abilities do not differ between men and women in a real-world, open-field environment. Behavioural Brain Research, 207(1), 125–137. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.046

Levy, L. J., Astur, R. S., & Frick, K. M. (2005). Men and women differ in object memory but not performance of a virtual radial maze. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(4), 853–862. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.119.4.853

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment (4 ed.). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Loring , D. W., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (1987). Memory assessment in neuropsychology: Theoretical considerations and practical utility. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9(4), 340-358. doi:10.1080/01688638708405055

Loring, D. W., Strauss, E., Hermann, B. P., Barr, W. B., Perrine, K., Trenerry , M. R., . . . Bowden, S. C. (2008). Differential neuropsychological test sensitivity to left temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(3), 394-400. doi:10.1017/S1355617708080582.

Meyers, J. E., & Meyers, K. R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test and recognition trial professional manual. New York: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Mitrushina, M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & DElia, L. F. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press. Chicago

Moffat, S. D., Hampson, E., & Hatzipantelis, M. (1998). Navigation in a “virtual†maze: Sex differences and correlation with psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(2), 73–87. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00104-9

Morris, R. (1981). Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learning and Motivation, 12(2), 239–260. doi:10.1016/0023-9690(81)90020-5

Morris, R. (1984). Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 11(1), 47–60. doi:10.1016/0165-0270(84)90007-4

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., . . . Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695–699. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

O'Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. New York: Oxford University Press.

Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une fingure complex. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 306-356.

Possin, K. L., Sanchez, P. E., Anderson-Bergman, C., Fernandez, R., Kerchner, G. A., Johnson, E. T., . . . Finkbeiner, S. (2016). Cross-species translation of the

Morris maze for Alzheimer’s disease. The Journal of Clinical Investigation, 126(2), 779-783. doi:10.1172/JCI78464.

Poreh, A. (2005). Analysis of mean learning of normal participants on the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test. Psychological Assessment, 17(2), 191-199. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.17.2.191

Poreh, A., Bezdicek, O., Korobkova, I., Levin, J. B., & Dines, P. (2016). The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test forced-choice recognition task: Base-rate data and norms. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 23(3), 155-161. doi:10.1080/23279095.2015.1027343

Poreh, A., Sultan, A., & Levin, J. (2012). The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: Normative data for the Arabic-speaking population and analysis of the differential influence of demographic variables. Psychology & Neuroscience, 5(1), 57-61. doi:10.3922/j.psns.2012.1.08

Pratt, J., Winchester, C., Dawson, N., & Morris, B. (2012). Advancing schizophrenia drug discovery: Optimizing rodent models to bridge the translational gap. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery, 11(7), 560-579. doi:10.1038/nrd3649

Rey, A. (1941). L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encéphalopathie traumatique. (Les problems.). [The psychological examination in cases of traumatic encepholopathy. Problems.]. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 215-285.

Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie [The psychological examination]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Shum, D., O'Gorman, J. G., & Eadie, K. (1999). Normative data for a new memory test: The Shum Visual Learning Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13(2), 121-135. doi:10.1076/clin.13.2.121.1967

Strauss, E., Sherman, E., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3 ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Tariq, S. H., Tumosa, N., Chibnall, J. T., Perry, M. H., & Morley, J. E. (2006). Comparison of the Saint Louis University Mental Status Examination and the Mini-Mental State Examination for detecting Dementia and Mild Neurocognitive Disorder - A pilot study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(11), 900–910. doi:10.1097/01.JGP.0000221510.33817.86

Taylor, E. M. (1959). Psychological appraisal of children with cerebral defects. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Troyer, A. K., & Wishart, H. A. (1997). A comparison of qualitative scoring systems for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11(4), 381-390. doi:10.1080/13854049708400467

Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138-146. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.138

Tulving, E., Mandler, G., & Baumal, R. (1964). Interaction of two sources of information in tachistoscopic word recognition. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 18(1), 62-71. doi:10.1037/h0083296

Van der Elst, W., Hurks, P., Wassenberg, R., Meijs, C., & Jolles, J. (2011). Animal Verbal Fluency and Design Fluency in school-aged children: Effects of age, sex, and mean level of parental education, and regression-based normative data. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(9), 1005–1015. doi:10.1080/13803395.2011.589509

Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale 3rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (2009). Wechsler Memory Scale 4th ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

Williams, M. T., Morford, L. L., Wood, S. L., Wallace, T. L., Fukumura, M., Broening, H. W., & Vorhees, C. V. (2003). Developmental d-methamphetamine treatment selectively induces spatial navigation impairments in reference memory in the Morris Water Maze while sparing working memory. Synapse, 48(3), 138-148. doi:10.1002/syn.10159

Willment, K. C., & Golby, A. (2013). Hemispheric lateralization interrupted: Material-specific memory deficits in temporal lobe epilepsy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1-8. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00546

Woolley, D. G., Vermaercke, B., de Beeck, H. O., Wagemans, J., Gantois, I., D’Hooge, R., . . . Wenderoth, N. (2010). Sex differences in human virtual water maze performance: Novel measures reveal the relative contribution of directional responding and spatial knowledge. Behavioural Brain Research, 208(2), 408–414. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.019

Yerkes, R. M. (1948). Psychological examining in the United States Army, 1921. In W. Dennis (Ed.), Readings in the history of psychology. (pp. 528-540). East Norwalk: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Downloads

Published

2017-05-01

Issue

Section

Neuropsychological Assessment